Can dogs understand metaphor? Maybe not. Maybe that kind of symbolic reasoning is beyond them. (Maybe.)

But they sure as hell understand metonymy.

To rehash:

Metaphor: “Deep down, he has a heart of gold.” (A comparison is made between a heart and the beauty, value and worth of gold, and the idea that this value is hidden.)

Metonymy: “I need a smart person. Where can I find some good brains?” (Brains are associated with intelligence. By substituting brains for people, I’m saying I don’t just need people, but people with good brains.)

Ronald Langacker describes metonymy as “basically a reference-point phenomenon” (30). He even gives us a fancy little diagram for these reference-point phenomenons (6):

Looking at the above diagram, if I say “I need a good pair of eyes” the concept “eyes” acts as the reference point that provides access to the real target: someone who’s good at finding things, or who has good vision. Eyes are a reference point for establishing mental contact with concepts of seeing, vision, looking, and so on.

Canine Metonymy

The quickest, easiest example of canine metonymy I can think of comes from the black labrador retriever I had as a child, Shadow.

If you told Shadow, “Hey Shadow… go get your leash,” he would go get his leash, but he would also get extremely excited (he was a lab, remember), because he knew that his leash was associated with going for a walk, and Shadow loved walks. It got to the point where it was cruel to tell him to go get his leash if you didn’t intend to take him for a walk then and there. If we described this to visitors, we had to spell everything out, just so Shadow wouldn’t get excited and — subsequently — disappointed.

The leash was a reference point capable, in Shadow’s mind, of establishing mental contact with the target, that is, using that leash to go for a walk outside. If you wanted to tell Shadow, “Hey, we’re going to go for a walk,” you could say that, and yes he would understand you (you had to spell out W-A-L-Ks too), but you could also say, “Alright, go get your leash” as a way of communicating that it was time for a walk.

Another very simple example of canine metonymy comes from my current dog, Chaucer. If I am working at home along in my office, sometimes Chaucer gets bored, and he wants to play. His favourite game is “bone.” Now, “bone” is a very unstructured game. Sometimes it involved tug-of-war, sometimes it involves fetch, sometimes it involves me chasing Chaucer. The one thing that says stable throughout is the use of Chaucer’s favourite squeeky toy, shaped like, you guessed it,  a purple bone. (Thankfully the bone no longer squeeks.)

In order to signify to me that he wants to play, and he wants to play some game involving his bone, Chaucer will bring his bone to me and drop it at me feet, then look at me expectantly. 

This is different from generic forms he uses of getting my attention, which he usually does by nudging my hand with his head (which makes it very difficult to ignore him and continue typing or using my computer). 

Dropping the bone at my feet signifies, Hey Mike, I have an amazing idea: let’s go out of this boring office and play with this bone in the living room! Wouldn’t that be fun!? 

I consider this example important to think about becuase it involves a dog not just understanding metonymization, but actively participating in it. 

Clicker Training Metonymy

Three more good examples of a kind of canine metonymy come from canine clicker training, which I’ve gotten into with Chaucer.

In clicker training, you use a small clicker device to produce a distinctive *click!* sound to mark a behavior as “good,” or as behavior that gets a reward. You only ever produce that distinctive *click!* when you are going to give the dog a treat. This helps you be a bit more precise with what you want from the dog. For example, when you’re training “sit,” you might give the *click!* the moment the dog’s butt touches the floor, and you don’t do it beforehand. This way, the dog knows that a sit isn’t complete until they’re sitting all the way. It helps with communication. (“Oh, that’s what you were looking for all this time!” thinks the confused dog.) 

There is nothing instinctive about this. It is a learned thing. In fact, when you begin with a clicker, you actually have to spend some time showing the dog that the click means a treat is coming. This is called “loading” the clicker.

Now, a *click!* sound is not a verbal word, but in this case, it is still metonymic sign. As Carita Paradis points out, “Metonymization is not restricted to linguistic communication but is also a construal in other communicative modes such as pointing and gesture” (67).

For a dog, the *click!* helps to bridge a longer conceptual chain of real-world relationships: [behavior] leads to [click] which leads to [reward]. Langacker talks about longer conceptual chains like this in volume I his Cognitive Grammar book. The *click!* is a reference point capable of establishing mental contact with the others: good behavior that gets a reward.

We can go a step further: Chaucer gets really excited when I take out any of his of his clickers, just like Shadow did when he was told to go get his leash. For Chaucer, you can take out a clicker device and wave it in your hands, and he will know what’s coming up next. He’ll sit in front of you, anxious with excitement, waiting to be told what behaviors will earn a click and thus a reward.

The clicker, then, is a reference point for Chaucer that provides access to multiple targets: an upcoming training session, which he loves (again, he is a lab, so the difficulty is actually in keeping him calm), the chance to earn treats, and more. If I take out the clicker and wave it, he knows what’s coming up.

Speaking of visual metonymies, one of my clickers is has an extra metonymic sign. Here is a special clicker we use, apparently called a “clik stik”:

Three more good examples of a kind of canine metonymy come from canine clicker training, which I’ve gotten into with my current dog, Chaucer.

In clicker training, you use a small clicker device to produce a distinctive *click!* sound to mark a behavior as “good,” or, more precisely, as behavior that gets a reward. You only ever produce that distinctive *click!* when you are going to give the dog a treat. This helps you be a bit more precise with what you want from the dog. For example, when you’re training “sit,” you might give the *click!* the moment their butt hits the floor, and you don’t do it beforehand. This way, the dog knows that a sit isn’t complete until they’re sitting all the way.

Now, a *click!* sound is not a verbal word, but it is still metonymic sign. As Carita Paradis points out, “Metonymization is not restricted to linguistic communication but is also a construal in other communicative modes such as pointing and gesture” (67).

(Slight digression: I would argue that metonymy is a figure of rhetoric, and therefore it needs a bit more than simple pointing or gestures or something to be considered figurative. That is, a figure doesn’t just signify; it has some degree of codification. An arrow is a Peircean indexical sign, but metonymy leverages indexicality as well as symbolization and codification. This sense of cognitive codification, for me, is the key difference between a pure indexical sign and metonymy. There’s a tonne of overlap, yes, but the two are not 100% the same.)

The *click!* helps to bridge a longer conceptual chain: [behavior]-[click]-[reward]. (Langacker talks about longer conceptual chains like this in his Cognitive Grammar book.) The *click!* is a reference point capable of establishing mental contact with two others: good behavior that gets a reward.

We can go a step further: Chaucer gets really excited when I take out any of his of his clickers, just like Shadow did when told to go get his leash. The clicker is a reference point that provides access to multiple targets: an upcoming training session, which he loves (again, he is a lab, so the difficulty is actually in keeping him calm), the chance to earn treats, and more. If I take out the clicker and wave it, he knows what’s coming up.

Speaking of visual metonymies, one of my clickers is has an extra metonymic sign. Here is a special clicker we use, apparently called a “clik stik”:

The point of the clik stik is that it extends, and the green ball at the end is a kind of indicator for where the dog should touch his nose. You can use it to do fun tricks just involving the “touch!” of the green ball, but you can also use it to lead the dog through more complex motions (um, Chaucer and I aren’t there yet).

The clik stik is an obvious indexical sign. For the dog, it indicates spatially and physically: “apply your nose here, touch this round dot.”

For Chaucer, who gets, shall we say, a bit over-eager, the clik stik is a metonym, as well, for that sort of training itself: Oh boy! Mike is going to want me to put my nose wherever he waves that stick, and it’s going to earn me treats!

Again, we have a more complex chain of cognitive associations: [clik stik] – [tricks involving touching the clic stik] – [*click!*] – [reward]

More than this, we have a meta metonym: a metonym signifying upcoming metonyms, or an indexical sign indicating upcoming indexical signs. For Chaucer, the clik stik itself is a metonym and semiotic indicator for the kind of training that involves metonyms and indexicality, that is, touching his nose to the spot where that little round ball on the end is phyiscally placed.

So, can dogs understand metonymy? Yes, of course.

Some Stuff I Mentioned:

Langacker, Ronald. “Reference-Point Constructions.” Cognitive Linguistics 4.1 (1993): 1–38. Print.

Paradis, Carita. “Metonymization: a Key Mechanism in Semantic Change.” Defining Metonymy in Cognitive Linguistics: Towards a Consensus View. Ed. Réka Benczes, Antonio Barcelona, and Francisco José Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2011. 61–87. Print.